Order:
  1.  11
    Spatial adaptation and aftereffect with optically transformed vision: Effects of active and passive responding and the relationship between test and exposure responses.G. Singer & R. H. Day - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (5):725.
  2.  45
    Basis of the horizontal-vertical illusion.G. C. Avery & R. H. Day - 1969 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 81 (2):376.
    No categories
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   9 citations  
  3.  16
    Apparent depth from progressive exposure of moving shadows: The kinetic depth effect in a narrow aperture.R. H. Day - 1989 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 27 (4):320-322.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   3 citations  
  4.  13
    Absence of the horizontal-vertical illusion in haptic space.R. H. Day & G. C. Avery - 1970 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 83 (1p1):172.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark   2 citations  
  5.  19
    Radial and tangential movement directions as determinants of the haptic illusion in an L figure.R. H. Day & T. S. Wong - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 87 (1):19.
  6.  26
    Relationship between the horizontal-vertical illusions for velocity and extent.G. C. Avery & R. H. Day - 1971 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 89 (1):22.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  7.  34
    What is the point of attempting to make a case for cognitive impenetrability of visual perception?Boris Crassini, Jack Broerse, R. H. Day, Christopher J. Best & W. A. Sparrow - 1999 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 22 (3):372-373.
    We question the usefulness of Pylyshyn's dichotomy between cognitively penetrable and cognitively impenetrable mechanisms as the basis for his distinction between cognition and early vision. This dichotomy is comparable to others that have been proposed in psychology prompting disputes that by their very nature could not be resolved. This fate is inevitable for Pylyshyn's thesis because of its reliance on internal representations and their interpretation. What is more fruitful in relation to this issue is not a difficult dichotomy, but a (...)
    Direct download (5 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  8.  4
    Application of the statistical theory to form perception.R. H. Day - 1956 - Psychological Review 63 (2):139-148.
    No categories
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  9.  4
    Apparent reversal (oscillation) of rotary motion in depth: An investigation and a general theory.R. H. Day & R. P. Power - 1965 - Psychological Review 72 (2):117-127.
  10.  56
    Images, depth cues, and cross-cultural differences in perception.R. H. Day - 1989 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 12 (1):78-79.
  11.  27
    Spatial aftereffects within and between kinesthesis and vision.R. H. Day & G. Singer - 1964 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 68 (4):337.
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  12.  5
    Some variant forms of the Poggendorff illusion and their implications for an explanation.R. H. Day & E. J. Stecher - 1992 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 30 (1):26-28.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  13.  10
    The Morinaga misalignment effect with circular stimulus elements.R. H. Day & R. T. Kasperczyk - 1984 - Bulletin of the Psychonomic Society 22 (3):193-196.
  14.  16
    The physiological basis of form perception in the peripheral retina.R. H. Day - 1957 - Psychological Review 64 (1):38-48.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  15.  21
    What is self-induced motor activity adapting to?R. H. Day - 1979 - Behavioral and Brain Sciences 2 (1):66-67.
  16.  12
    Correlation between visual and kinesthetic spatial aftereffects.A. A. Landauer, G. Singer & R. H. Day - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 72 (6):892.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  17.  66
    William James and the evolution of consciousness.Mark Nielsen & R. H. Day - 1999 - Journal of Theoretical and Philosophical Psychology 19 (1):90-113.
    Despite having been relegated to the realm of superstition during the dominant years of behaviorism, the investigation and discussion of consciousness has again become scientifically defensible. However, attempts at describing animal consciousness continue to be criticized for lacking independent criteria that identify the presence or absence of the phenomenon. William James recognized that mental traits are subject to the same evolutionary processes as are physical characteristics and must therefore be represented in differing levels of complexity throughout the animal kingdom. James's (...)
    Direct download (6 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  18.  9
    Interlimb and interjoint transfer of a kinesthetic spatial aftereffect.G. Singer & R. H. Day - 1966 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 71 (1):109.
  19.  21
    Temporal determinants of a kinesthetic aftereffect.G. Singer & R. H. Day - 1965 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 69 (4):343.
    No categories
    Direct download (4 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark  
  20.  15
    Development and dissipation of a visual spatial aftereffect from prolonged head tilt.N. J. Wade & R. H. Day - 1968 - Journal of Experimental Psychology 76 (3p1):439.
    Direct download (3 more)  
     
    Export citation  
     
    Bookmark